Auditory processing disorder | |
---|---|
Other names | Central auditory processing disorder |
Specialty | Audiology |
Auditory processing disorder and rarely known as King-Kopetzky syndrome or auditory disability with normal hearing (ADN) is an umbrella term for a variety of disorders that affect the way the brain processes auditory information.[1] Individuals with APD usually have normal structure and function of the outer, middle, and inner ear (peripheral hearing). However, they cannot process the information they hear in the same way as others do, which leads to difficulties in recognizing and interpreting sounds, especially the sounds composing speech. It is thought that these difficulties arise from dysfunction in the central nervous system.
- The Listening Program. With individualized programming the best possible outcome is optimized with options for Classical music only, or music blended with beautiful sounds of nature. SPECTRUM is a foundational program created for younger children and adults with sensory sensitivities. SPECTRUM is typically most suited to; Those on the Autism Spectrum.
- Program Activity. The program entails 1 to 2 fifteen-minute listening sessions a day, five days per week. The average program length is eight weeks with many people finding it advantageous to repeat the program once or several times to keep their auditory system in peak condition. Listening is done through a pair of high-quality headphones and a CD Player.
However, while the quieter homeschool environment is helpful, auditory processing affects more than just listening. It also impacts reading and focus. And so a homeschool program for auditory processing disorder, along with your child’s Language Arts curriculum, should be part of your child’s daily routine.
The American Academy of Audiology notes that APD is diagnosed by difficulties in one or more auditory processes known to reflect the function of the central auditory nervous system.[1] It can affect both children and adults. Although the actual prevalence is currently unknown, it has been estimated to be 2–7% in children in US and UK populations.[2] APD can continue into adulthood. Cooper and Gates (1991) estimated the prevalence of adult APD to be 10 to 20%. It has been reported that males are twice as likely to be affected by the disorder as females,[3][4] and that prevalence is higher in the elderly and increases with age.[5]
- 1Signs and symptoms
- 2Causes
- 3Diagnosis
Signs and symptoms[edit]
Many people experience problems with learning and day-to-day tasks with difficulties over time. Adults with this disorder[6] can experience the signs and symptoms below:
- talk louder than necessary
- have trouble remembering a list or sequence
- often need words or sentences repeated
- have poor ability to memorize information learned by listening
- interpret words too literally
- need assistance hearing clearly in noisy environments
- rely on accommodation and modification strategies
- find or request a quiet work space away from others
- request written material when attending oral presentations
- ask for directions to be given one step at a time
Relation to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder[edit]
It has been discovered that APD and ADHD present overlapping symptoms. Below is a ranked order of behavioral symptoms that are most frequently observed in each disorder. Professionals evaluated the overlap of symptoms between the two disorders. The order below is of symptoms that are almost always observed.[7] This chart proves that although the symptoms listed are different, it is easy to get confused between many of them.
ADHD | APD |
---|---|
1. Inattentive | 1. Difficult hearing in background noise |
2. Distracted | 2. Difficulty following oral instructions |
3. Hyperactive | 3. Poor listening skills |
4. Fidgety or restless | 4. Academic difficulties |
5. Hasty or impulsive | 5. Poor auditory association skills |
6. Interrupts or intrudes | 6. Distracted |
7. Inattentive |
There is a high rate of co-occurrence between ADHD and APD. An article published in 1994 showed that 84% of children with APD have confirmed or suspected ADHD. Co-occurrence between ADHD and APD is 41% for children with confirmed diagnosis of ADHD, and 43% for children suspected of having ADHD.[8]more recently published data is needed to support or refute this statement.
Relation to specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia[edit]
There has been considerable debate over the relationship between APD and Specific language impairment (SLI).
SLI is diagnosed when a child has difficulties with understanding or producing spoken language for no obvious cause. The problems cannot be explained in terms of peripheral hearing loss. The child is typically late in starting to talk, and may have problems in producing speech sounds clearly, and in producing or understanding complex sentences. Some theoretical accounts of SLI regard it as the result of auditory processing problems.[9][10] However, this view of SLI is not universally accepted, and others regard the main difficulties in SLI as stemming from problems with higher-level aspects of language processing. Where a child has both auditory and language problems, it can be hard to sort out cause-and-effect.[10]
Similarly with developmental dyslexia, there has been considerable interest in the idea that for some children reading problems are downstream consequences of difficulties in rapid auditory processing. Again, cause and effect can be hard to unravel. This is one reason why some experts have recommended using non-verbal auditory tests to diagnose APD.[11] Specifically regarding the neurological factors of dyslexia, the disorder has been linked to polymicrogyria which causes cell migrational problems. This relates to APD because children that have polymicrogyri almost always present deficits on APD testing.[2] It has also been suggested that APD may be related to cluttering,[12] a fluency disorder marked by word and phrase repetitions.
It has been found that a higher than expected proportion of individuals diagnosed with SLI and dyslexia on the basis of language and reading tests also perform poorly on tests in which auditory processing skills are tested.[13] APD can be assessed using tests that involve identifying, repeating or discriminating speech, and a child may do poorly because of primary language problems.[14] In a study comparing children with a diagnosis of dyslexia and those with a diagnosis of APD, they found the two groups could not be distinguished.[10][15][16] obtained similar findings in studies comparing children diagnosed with SLI or APD.[17][18] The two groups had very similar profiles. This raises the worrying possibility that the diagnosis that a child receives may be largely a function of the specialist they see: the same child who would be diagnosed with APD by an audiologist may be diagnosed with SLI by a speech-language therapist or with dyslexia by a psychologist.[11]
Causes[edit]
Acquired[edit]
Acquired APD can be caused by any damage to or dysfunction of the central auditory nervous system and can cause auditory processing problems.[19][20] For an overview of neurological aspects of APD, see Griffiths.[21]
Genetics[edit]
Some studies indicated an increased prevalence of a family history of hearing impairment in these patients. The pattern of results is suggestive that Auditory Processing Disorder may be related to conditions of autosomal dominant inheritance.[22][23][24] The ability to listen to and comprehend multiple messages at the same time is a trait that is heavily influenced by our genes say federal researchers.[25] These 'short circuits in the wiring' sometimes run in families or result from a difficult birth, just like any learning disability.[26] Auditory processing disorder can be associated with conditions affected by genetic traits, such as various developmental disorders. Inheritance of Auditory Processing Disorder refers to whether the condition is inherited from your parents or 'runs' in families.[27] Central auditory processing disorder may be hereditary neurological traits from the mother or the father.[citation needed]
Developmental[edit]
In the majority of cases of developmental APD, the cause is unknown. An exception is acquired epileptic aphasia or Landau-Kleffner syndrome, where a child's development regresses, with language comprehension severely affected.[28] The child is often thought to be deaf, but normal peripheral hearing is found. In other cases, suspected or known causes of APD in children include delay in myelin maturation,[29] ectopic (misplaced) cells in the auditory cortical areas,[30] or genetic predisposition.[31] In a family with autosomal dominantepilepsy, seizures which affected the left temporal lobe seemed to cause problems with auditory processing.[32] In another extended family with a high rate of APD, genetic analysis showed a haplotype in chromosome 12 that fully co-segregated with language impairment.[33]
Hearing begins in utero, but the central auditory system continues to develop for at least the first decade.[34] There is considerable interest in the idea that disruption to hearing during a sensitive period may have long-term consequences for auditory development.[35] One study showed thalamocortical connectivity in vitro was associated with a time sensitive developmental window and required a specific cell adhesion molecule (lcam5) for proper brain plasticity to occur.[36] This points to connectivity between the thalamus and cortex shortly after being able to hear (in vitro) as at least one critical period for auditory processing. Another study showed that rats reared in a single tone environment during critical periods of development had permanently impaired auditory processing.[37] ‘Bad’ auditory experiences, such as temporary deafness by cochlear removal in rats leads to neuron shrinkage.[34] In a study looking at attention in APD patients, children with one ear blocked developed a strong right-ear advantage but were not able to modulate that advantage during directed-attention tasks.[38]
In the 1980s and 1990s, there was considerable interest in the role of chronic Otitis media (middle ear disease or 'glue ear') in causing APD and related language and literacy problems. Otitis media with effusion is a very common childhood disease that causes a fluctuating conductive hearing loss, and there was concern this may disrupt auditory development if it occurred during a sensitive period.[39] Consistent with this, in a sample of young children with chronic ear infections recruited from a hospital otolargyngology department, increased rates of auditory difficulties were found later in childhood.[40] However, this kind of study will suffer from sampling bias because children with otitis media will be more likely to be referred to hospital departments if they are experiencing developmental difficulties. Compared with hospital studies, epidemiological studies, which assesses a whole population for otitis media and then evaluate outcomes, have found much weaker evidence for long-term impacts of otitis media on language outcomes.[41]
Somatic[edit]
It seems that somatic anxiety (that is, physical symptoms of anxiety such as butterflies in the stomach or cotton mouth) and situations of stress may be determinants of speech-hearing disability.[42][43]
Diagnosis[edit]
Questionnaires can be used for the identification of persons with possible auditory processing disorders, as these address common problems of listening. They can help in the decision for pursuing clinical evaluation. One of the most common listening problems is speech recognition in the presence of background noise.[44][45] According to the respondents who participated in a study by Neijenhuis, de Wit, and Luinge (2017),[46] the following symptoms are characteristic in children with listening difficulties, and they are typically problematic with adolescents and adults. They include:
- Difficulty hearing in noise
- Auditory attention problems
- Better understanding in one on one situations
- Difficulties in noise localization
- Difficulties in remembering oral information
According to the New Zealand Guidelines on Auditory Processing Disorders (2017)[47] a checklist of key symptoms of APD or comorbidities that can be used to identify individuals who should be referred for audiological and APD assessment includes, among others:
- Difficulty following spoken directions unless they are brief and simple
- Difficulty attending to and remembering spoken information
- Slowness in processing spoken information
- Difficulty understanding in the presence of other sounds
- Overwhelmed by complex or “busy” auditory environments e.g. classrooms, shopping malls
- Poor listening skills
- Insensitivity to tone of voice or other nuances of speech
- Acquired brain injury
- History of frequent or persistent middle ear disease (otitis media, ‘glue ear’).
- Difficulty with language, reading or spelling
- Suspicion or diagnosis of dyslexia
- Suspicion or diagnosis of language disorder or delay
Finally, the New Zealand guidelines state that behavioral checklists and questionnaires should only be used to provide guidance for referrals, for information gathering (for example, prior to assessment or as outcome measures for interventions), and as measures to describe the functional impact of auditory processing disorder. They are not designed for the purpose of diagnosing auditory processing disorders. The New Zealand guidelines indicate that a number of questionnaires have been developed to identify children who might benefit from evaluation of their problems in listening. Examples of available questionnaires include the Fisher’s Auditory Problems Checklist (1976[48]), the Children’s Auditory Performance Scale (1998[49]), the Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk (1989[50]), and the Auditory Processing Domains Questionnaire (O’Haraa and Mealings, 2017[51]) among others. All of the previous questionnaires were designed for children and none are useful for adolescents and adults.
The University of Cincinnati Auditory Processing Inventory (UCAPI) (Keith, Tektas and Ramsey, 2018[52]) was designed for use with adolescents and adults seeking testing for evaluation of problems with listening and/or to be used following diagnosis of an auditory processing disorder to determine the subject’s status. Following a model described by Zoppo et al. (2015[53]) a 34-item questionnaire was developed that investigates auditory processing abilities in each of the six common areas of complaint in APD (listening and concentration, understanding speech, following spoken instructions, attention, and other.) The final questionnaire was standardized on normally achieving young adults ranging from 18 to 27 years of age. Validation data was acquired from subjects with language-learning or auditory processing disorders who were either self-reported or confirmed by diagnostic testing. A UCAPI total score is calculated by combining the totals from the six listening conditions and provides an overall value to categorize listening abilities. Additionally, analysis of the scores from the six listening conditions provides an auditory profile for the subject. Each listening condition can then be utilized by the professional in making recommendation for diagnosing problem of learning through listening and treatment decisions. The UCAPI provides information on listening problems in various populations that can aid examiners in making recommendations for assessment and management.
APD is a difficult disorder to detect and diagnose. The subjective symptoms that lead to an evaluation for APD include an intermittent inability to process verbal information, leading the person to guess to fill in the processing gaps. There may also be disproportionate problems with decoding speech in noisy environments.
APD has been defined anatomically in terms of the integrity of the auditory areas of the nervous system.[54] However, children with symptoms of APD typically have no evidence of neurological disease and the diagnosis is made on the basis of performance on behavioral auditory tests. Auditory processing is 'what we do with what we hear',[55] and in APD there is a mismatch between peripheral hearing ability (which is typically normal) and ability to interpret or discriminate sounds. Thus in those with no signs of neurological impairment, APD is diagnosed on the basis of auditory tests. There is, however, no consensus as to which tests should be used for diagnosis, as evidenced by the succession of task force reports that have appeared in recent years. The first of these occurred in 1996.[56] This was followed by a conference organized by the American Academy of Audiology.[57] Experts attempting to define diagnostic criteria have to grapple with the problem that a child may do poorly on an auditory test for reasons other than poor auditory perception: for instance, failure could be due to inattention, difficulty in coping with task demands, or limited language ability. In an attempt to rule out at least some of these factors, the American Academy of Audiology conference explicitly advocated that for APD to be diagnosed, the child must have a modality-specific problem, i.e. affecting auditory but not visual processing. However, an ASHA committee subsequently rejected modality-specificity as a defining characteristic of auditory processing disorders.[58]
Definitions[edit]
The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) published '(Central) Auditory Processing Disorders' in January 2005 as an update to the 'Central Auditory Processing: Current Status of Research and Implications for Clinical Practice (ASHA, 1996)'.[56] The American Academy of Audiology has released more current practice guidelines related to the disorder.[1] ASHA formally defines APA as 'a difficulty in the efficiency and effectiveness by which the central nervous system (CNS) utilizes auditory information.'[59]
In 2011, the British Society of Audiology published 'best practice guidelines'.[60]
Auditory processing disorder can be developmental or acquired. It may result from ear infections, head injuries or neurodevelopmental delays that affect processing of auditory information. This can include problems with: '...sound localization and lateralization (see also binaural fusion); auditory discrimination; auditory pattern recognition; temporal aspects of audition, including temporal integration, temporal discrimination (e.g., temporal gap detection), temporal ordering, and temporal masking; auditory performance in competing acoustic signals (including dichotic listening); and auditory performance with degraded acoustic signals'.[58]
The Committee of UK Medical Professionals Steering the UK Auditory Processing Disorder Research Program have developed the following working definition of Auditory Processing Disorder: 'APD results from impaired neural function and is characterized by poor recognition, discrimination, separation, grouping, localization, or ordering of speech sounds. It does not solely result from a deficit in general attention, language or other cognitive processes.'[61]
Types of testing[edit]
1. The SCAN-C[62]for children and SCAN-A[63] for adolescents and adults are the most common tools for screening and diagnosing APD in the USA. Both tests are standardized on a large number of subjects and include validation data on subjects with auditory processing disorders. The test batteries include screening tests: norm-based criterion-referenced scores; diagnostic tests: scaled scores, percentile ranks and ear advantage scores for all tests except the Gap Detection test. The four tests include four subsets on which the subject scores are derived include: discrimination of monaurally presented single words against background noise (speech in noise), acoustically degraded single words (filtered words), dichotically presented single words and sentences.
2. Random Gap Detection Test (RGDT) is also a standardized test. It assesses an individual’s gap detection threshold of tones and white noise. The exam includes stimuli at four different frequencies (500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz) and white noise clicks of 50 ms duration. It is a useful test because it provides an index of auditory temporal resolution. In children, an overall gap detection threshold greater than 20 ms means they have failed and may have an auditory processing disorder based on abnormal perception of sound in the time domain.[64][65]
3. Gaps in Noise Test (GIN) also measures temporal resolution by testing the patient's gap detection threshold in white noise.[66]
4. Pitch Patterns Sequence Test (PPT) and Duration Patterns Sequence Test (DPT) measure auditory pattern identification. The PPS has s series of three tones presented at either of two pitches (high or low). Meanwhile, the DPS has a series of three tones that vary in duration rather than pitch (long or short). Patients are then asked to describe the pattern of pitches presented.[67]
5. Masking Level Difference (MLD) at 500 Hz measures overlapping temporal processing, binaural processing, and low-redundancy by measuring the difference in threshold of an auditory stimulus when a masking noise is presented in and out of phase.[68]
Modality-specificity and controversies[edit]
The issue of modality-specificity has led to considerable debate among experts in this field. Cacace and McFarland have argued that APD should be defined as a modality-specific perceptual dysfunction that is not due to peripheral hearing loss.[69][70] They criticise more inclusive conceptualizations of APD as lacking diagnostic specificity.[71] A requirement for modality-specificity could potentially avoid including children whose poor auditory performance is due to general factors such as poor attention or memory.[69][70] Others, however, have argued that a modality-specific approach is too narrow, and that it would miss children who had genuine perceptual problems affecting both visual and auditory processing. It is also impractical, as audiologists do not have access to standardized tests that are visual analogs of auditory tests. The debate over this issue remains unresolved. It is clear, however, that a modality-specific approach will diagnose fewer children with APD than a modality-general one, and that the latter approach runs a risk of including children who fail auditory tests for reasons other than poor auditory processing. Although modality-specific testing has been advocated for well over a decade, to date no tests have been published which would allow audiologists to perform a modality-specific evaluation (i.e., no clinical versions of visual analogs to auditory processing tests exist).[citation needed]
Another controversy concerns the fact that most traditional tests of APD use verbal materials.[11] The British Society of Audiology[60] has embraced Moore's (2006) recommendation that tests for APD should assess processing of non-speech sounds.[11] The concern is that if verbal materials are used to test for APD, then children may fail because of limited language ability. An analogy may be drawn with trying to listen to sounds in a foreign language. It is much harder to distinguish between sounds or to remember a sequence of words in a language you do not know well: the problem is not an auditory one, but rather due to lack of expertise in the language.
In recent years there have been additional criticisms of some popular tests for diagnosis of APD. Tests that use tape-recorded American English have been shown to over-identify APD in speakers of other forms of English.[72] Performance on a battery of non-verbal auditory tests devised by the Medical Research Council's Institute of Hearing Research was found to be heavily influenced by non-sensory task demands, and indices of APD had low reliability when this was controlled for.[73][74] This research undermines the validity of APD as a distinct entity in its own right and suggests that the use of the term 'disorder' itself is unwarranted. In a recent review of such diagnostic issues, it was recommended that children with suspected auditory processing impairments receive a holistic psychometric assessment including general intellectual ability, auditory memory, and attention, phonological processing, language, and literacy.[75] The authors state that 'a clearer understanding of the relative contributions of perceptual and non-sensory, unimodal and supramodal factors to performance on psychoacoustic tests may well be the key to unravelling the clinical presentation of these individuals.'[75]
Depending on how it is defined, APD may share common symptoms with ADD/ADHD, specific language impairment, and autism spectrum disorders. A review showed substantial evidence for atypical processing of auditory information in children with autism.[76] Dawes and Bishop noted how specialists in audiology and speech-language pathology often adopted different approaches to child assessment, and they concluded their review as follows: 'We regard it as crucial that these different professional groups work together in carrying out assessment, treatment and management of children and undertaking cross-disciplinary research.'[14] In practice, this seems rare.
To ensure that APD is correctly diagnosed, the examiners must differentiate APD from other disorders with similar symptoms. Factors that should be taken into account during the diagnosis are: attention, auditory neuropathy, fatigue, hearing and sensitivity, intellectual and developmental age, medications, motivation, motor skills, native language and language experience, response strategies and decision-making style, and visual acuity.[77]
It should also be noted that children under the age of seven cannot be evaluated correctly because their language and auditory processes are still developing. In addition, the presence of APD cannot be evaluated when a child's primary language is not English.[78]
Characteristics[edit]
The National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders[79] state that children with Auditory Processing Disorder often:
- have trouble paying attention to and remembering information presented orally, and may cope better with visually acquired information
- have problems carrying out multi-step directions given orally; need to hear only one direction at a time
- have poor listening skills
- need more time to process information
- have low academic performance
- have behavior problems
- have language difficulties (e.g., they confuse syllable sequences and have problems developing vocabulary and understanding language)
- have difficulty with reading, comprehension, spelling, and vocabulary
APD can manifest as problems determining the direction of sounds, difficulty perceiving differences between speech sounds and the sequencing of these sounds into meaningful words, confusing similar sounds such as 'hat' with 'bat', 'there' with 'where', etc. Fewer words may be perceived than were actually said, as there can be problems detecting the gaps between words, creating the sense that someone is speaking unfamiliar or nonsense words. In addition, it is common for APD to cause speech errors involving the distortion and substitution of consonant sounds.[80] Those suffering from APD may have problems relating what has been said with its meaning, despite obvious recognition that a word has been said, as well as repetition of the word. Background noise, such as the sound of a radio, television or a noisy bar can make it difficult to impossible to understand speech, since spoken words may sound distorted either into irrelevant words or words that don't exist, depending on the severity of the auditory processing disorder.[81] Using a telephone can be problematic for someone with auditory processing disorder, in comparison with someone with normal auditory processing, due to low quality audio, poor signal, intermittent sounds and the chopping of words.[58] Many who have auditory processing disorder subconsciously develop visual coping strategies, such as lip reading, reading body language, and eye contact, to compensate for their auditory deficit, and these coping strategies are not available when using a telephone.
As noted above, the status of APD as a distinct disorder has been queried, especially by speech-language pathologists[82] and psychologists,[83] who note the overlap between clinical profiles of children diagnosed with APD and those with other forms of specific learning disability. Many audiologists, however, would dispute that APD is just an alternative label for dyslexia, SLI, or ADHD, noting that although it often co-occurs with these conditions, it can be found in isolation.[84]
Subcategories[edit]
Based on sensitized measures of auditory dysfunction and on psychological assessment, patients can be subdivided into seven subcategories:[85]
- middle ear dysfunction
- mild cochlear pathology
- central/medial olivocochlear efferent system (MOCS) auditory dysfunction
- purely psychological problems
- multiple auditory pathologies
- combined auditory dysfunction and psychological problems
- unknown
Different subgroups may represent different pathogenic and aetiological factors. Thus, subcategorization provides further understanding of the basis of Auditory Processing Disorder, and hence may guide the rehabilitative management of these patients.This was suggested by Professor Dafydd Stephens and F Zhao at the Welsh Hearing Institute, Cardiff University.[86]
Treatment[edit]
Treatment of APD typically focuses on three primary areas: changing learning environment, developing higher-order skills to compensate for the disorder, and remediation of the auditory deficit itself.[87] However, there is a lack of well-conducted evaluations of intervention using randomized controlled trial methodology. Most evidence for effectiveness adopts weaker standards of evidence, such as showing that performance improves after training. This does not control for possible influences of practice, maturation, or placebo effects. Recent research has shown that practice with basic auditory processing tasks (i.e. auditory training) may improve performance on auditory processing measures[88][89] and phonemic awareness measures.[90] Changes after auditory training have also been recorded at the physiological level.[91][92] Many of these tasks are incorporated into computer-based auditory training programs such as Earobics and Fast ForWord, an adaptive software available at home and in clinics worldwide, but overall, evidence for effectiveness of these computerised interventions in improving language and literacy is not impressive.[93] One small-scale uncontrolled study reported successful outcomes for children with APD using auditory training software.[94]
Treating additional issues related to APD can result in success. For example, treatment for phonological disorders (difficulty in speech) can result in success in terms of both the phonological disorder as well as APD. In one study, speech therapy improved auditory evoked potentials (a measure of brain activity in the auditory portions of the brain).[95]
While there is evidence that language training is effective for improving APD, there is no current research supporting the following APD treatments:
- Auditory Integration Training typically involves a child attending two 30-minute sessions per day for ten days.[96]
- Lindamood-Bell Learning Processes (particularly, the Visualizing and Verbalizing program)
- Physical activities that require frequent crossing of the midline (e.g., occupational therapy)
- Sound Field Amplification
- Neuro-Sensory Educational Therapy
- Neurofeedback
However, use of a FM transmitter has been shown to produce significant improvements over time with children.
History[edit]
Samuel J. Kopetzky, who first described the condition in 1948. P. F. King, first discussed the aetiological factors behind it in 1954.[97] Helmer Myklebust's 1954 study, 'Auditory Disorders in Children'.[98] suggested auditory processing disorder was separate from language learning difficulties. His work sparked interest in auditory deficits after acquired brain lesions affecting the temporal lobes[99][100] and led to additional work looking at the physiological basis of auditory processing,[101] but it was not until the late seventies and early eighties that research began on APD in depth.In 1977, the first conference on the topic of APD was organized by Robert W. Keith, Ph.D. at the University of Cincinnati. The proceedings of that conference was published by Grune and Stratton under the title 'Central Auditory Dysfunction' (Keith RW Ed.) That conference started a new series of studies focusing on APD in children.[102][103][104][105][106] Virtually all tests currently used to diagnose APD originate from this work. These early researchers also invented many of the auditory training approaches, including interhemispheric transfer training and interaural intensity difference training. This period gave us a rough understanding of the causes and possible treatment options for APD.Much of the work in the late nineties and 2000s has been looking to refining testing, developing more sophisticated treatment options, and looking for genetic risk factors for APD. Scientists have worked on improving behavioral tests of auditory function, neuroimaging, electroacoustic, and electrophysiologic testing.[107][108] Working with new technology has led to a number of software programs for auditory training.[109][110] With global awareness of mental disorders and increasing understanding of neuroscience, auditory processing is more in the public and academic consciousness than ever before.[111][112][113][114][115]
See also[edit]
References[edit]
- ^ abcAmerican Academy of Audiology. 'Clinical Practice Guidelines: Diagnosis, Treatment and Management of Children and Adults with Central Auditory'(PDF). Retrieved 16 January 2017.
- ^ abChermak, Gail; Musiek, Frank (2014). Handbook of central auditory processing disorder. comprehensive intervention (2 ed.). San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing. ISBN978-1-59756-562-2.
- ^La Trobe University. '(C)APD'. Retrieved 14 November 2010.
- ^Musiek, Frank; Gail, Chermak (2007). Handbook of central auditory processing disorder [auditory neuroscience and diagnosis]. Plural Publishing. p. 448. ISBN978-1-59756-056-6.
- ^(Cooper JC Jr., Gates GA. Hearing in the elderly – the Framingham cohort, 1983–1985: Part II. Prevalence of central auditory processing disorders. Ear Hear. 1991;12(5):304–311)
- ^National Center for Learning Disabilities | NCLD.org – NCLD. National Center for Learning Disabilities | NCLD.org – NCLD. N.p., n.d. Web. 19 Nov. 2014.
- ^Chermak, Gail D.; Somers, Erin K.; Seikel, J. Anthony (1998). 'Behavioral signs of central auditory processing disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder'. Journal of American Academy of Audiology: 78–84.
- ^RICCIO, CYNTHIA A.; HYND, GEORGE W.; COHEN, MORRIS J.; HALL, JOSH; MOLT, LAWRENCE (July 1994). 'Comorbidity of Central Auditory Processing Disorder and Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder'. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 33 (6): 849–857. doi:10.1097/00004583-199407000-00011. PMID8083142.
- ^Miller, C. A. (2011). 'Auditory processing theories of language disorders: Past, present, and future'. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 42 (3): 309–319. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0040). PMID21757567.
- ^ abcFerguson, M. A.; Hall, R. L.; Riley, A; Moore, D. R. (2011). 'Communication, listening, cognitive and speech perception skills in children with auditory processing disorder (APD) or specific language impairment (SLI)'. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 54 (1): 211–227. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0167). PMID20689032.
- ^ abcdMoore, David R. (2006). 'Auditory processing disorder (APD): Definition, diagnosis, neural basis, and intervention'. Audiological Medicine. 4 (1): 4–11. doi:10.1080/16513860600568573.
- ^Pindzola, Rebekah H.; Haynes, William O.; Moran, Michael J. (2006). Communication disorders in the classroom: an introduction for professionals in school setting. Boston: Jones and Bartlett Publishers. p. 251. ISBN978-0-7637-2743-7. OCLC59401841.
- ^Moore, David R. (July 2007). 'Auditory processing disorders: Acquisition and treatment'. Journal of Communication Disorders. 40 (4): 295–304. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2007.03.005. PMID17467002.
- ^ abDawes P, Bishop D (2009). 'Auditory processing disorder in relation to developmental disorders of language, communication and attention: a review and critique'. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 44 (4): 440–65. doi:10.1080/13682820902929073. PMID19925352.
- ^Dawes, P; Bishop, D (2010). 'Psychometric profile of children with auditory processing disorder (APD) and children with dyslexia'. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 95 (6): 432–436. doi:10.1136/adc.2009.170118. PMC3576918. PMID20501538.
- ^Miller, C.A.; Wagstaff, D.A. (2011). 'Behavioral profiles associated with auditory processing disorder and specific language impairment'. Journal of Communication Disorders. 44 (6): 745–763. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.04.001. PMC3174343. PMID21636094.
- ^Corriveau K, Pasquini E, Goswami U (June 2007). 'Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: a new look at an old hypothesis'. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50 (3): 647–66. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/046). PMID17538107.
- ^Dlouha O, Novak A, Vokral J (June 2007). 'Central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Central auditory tests'. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 71 (6): 903–7. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2007.02.012. PMID17382411.
- ^Musiek FE, Chermak GD, Weihing J, Zappulla M, Nagle S (June 2011). 'Diagnostic accuracy of established central auditory processing test batteries in patients with documented brain lesions'. J Am Acad Audiol. 22 (6): 342–58. doi:10.3766/jaaa.22.6.4. PMID21864472.
- ^Lew HL, Weihing J, Myers PJ, Pogoda TK, Goodrich GL (2010). 'Dual sensory impairment (DSI) in traumatic brain injury (TBI)--An emerging interdisciplinary challenge'. NeuroRehabilitation. 26 (3): 213–22. doi:10.3233/NRE-2010-0557. PMID20448311.
- ^Griffiths, T. D. (2002). 'Central auditory pathologies'. British Medical Bulletin. 63 (1): 107–120. doi:10.1093/bmb/63.1.107. PMID12324387.
- ^Stephens D, Zhao F (March 2000). 'The role of a family history in King Kopetzky Syndrome (obscure auditory dysfunction)'. Acta Otolaryngol. 120 (2): 197–200. doi:10.1080/000164800750000900. PMID11603771.
- ^Liu XZ, Xu L, Newton V. 'Audiometric configuration in non-syndromic genetic hearing loss'. J Audiol Med. 3: 99–106.
- ^Van Camp G, Willems PJ, Smith RJ (1997). 'Non-syndromic hearing impairment: unparalleled heterogeneity'. Am J Genet. 60: 758–64.
- ^('Genetics Influence Auditory Processing.' Psych Central.com. N.p., n.d. Web. 02 Dec. 2014.)
- ^(NCLD.org – NCLD.' National Center for Learning Disabilities | NCLD.org.)
- ^('Inheritance and Genetics of Auditory Processing Disorder.' – RightDiagnosis.com. N.p., n.d. Web. 02 Dec. 2014.)
- ^Fandiño M, Connolly M, Usher L, Palm S, Kozak FK (January 2011). 'Landau-Kleffner syndrome: a rare auditory processing disorder series of cases and review of the literature'. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 75 (1): 33–8. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2010.10.001. PMID21074868.
- ^Weihing, Jeff; Musiek, Frank (2007). '15 Dichotic Interaural Intensity Difference (DIID)'. In Ross-Swain, Deborah; Geffner, Donna S (eds.). Auditory Processing Disorders: Assessment, Management and Treatment. Plural Publishing Inc. ISBN978-1-59756-107-5. OCLC255602759.
- ^Boscariol M, Garcia VL, Guimarães CA, et al. (April 2010). 'Auditory processing disorder in perisylvian syndrome'. Brain Dev. 32 (4): 299–304. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2009.04.002. PMID19410403.
- ^Bamiou DE, Campbell NG, Musiek FE, et al. (April 2007). 'Auditory and verbal working memory deficits in a child with congenital aniridia due to a PAX6 mutation'. Int J Audiol. 46 (4): 196–202. doi:10.1080/14992020601175952. PMID17454233.
- ^Pisano T, Marini C, Brovedani P, Brizzolara D, Pruna D, Mei D, Moro F, Cianchetti C, Guerrini R (January 2005). 'Abnormal phonologic processing in familial lateral temporal lobe epilepsy due to a new LGI1 mutation'. Epilepsia. 46 (1): 118–23. doi:10.1111/j.0013-9580.2005.26304.x. PMID15660777.
- ^Addis L, Friederici AD, Kotz SA, Sabisch B, Barry J, Richter N, Ludwig AA, Rübsamen R, Albert FW, Pääbo S, Newbury DF, Monaco AP (August 2010). 'A locus for an auditory processing deficit and language impairment in an extended pedigree maps to 12p13.31-q14.3'. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 9 (6): 545–61. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00583.x. PMC2948670. PMID20345892.
- ^ abMoore DR (2002). 'Auditory development and the role of experience'. British Medical Bulletin. 63: 171–81. doi:10.1093/bmb/63.1.171. PMID12324392.
- ^Thai-Van H, Veuillet E, Norena A, Guiraud J, Collet L (March 2010). 'Plasticity of tonotopic maps in humans: influence of hearing loss, hearing aids and cochlear implants'. Acta Otolaryngol. 130 (3): 333–7. doi:10.3109/00016480903258024. PMID19845491.
- ^Barkat TR, Polley DB, Hensch TK (September 2011). 'A critical period for auditory thalamocortical connectivity'. Nature Neuroscience. 14 (9): 1189–94. doi:10.1038/nn.2882. PMC3419581. PMID21804538.
- ^Han YK, Köver H, Insanally MN, Semerdjian JH, Bao S (September 2007). 'Early experience impairs perceptual discrimination'. Nature Neuroscience. 10 (9): 1191–7. doi:10.1038/nn1941. PMID17660815.
- ^Asbjørnsen A, Holmefjord A, Reisaeter S, Møller P, Klausen O, Prytz B, Boliek C, Obrzut JE (July 2000). 'Lasting auditory attention impairment after persistent middle ear infections: a dichotic listening study'. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 42 (7): 481–6. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2000.tb00352.x. PMID10972421.
- ^Whitton JP, Polley DB (October 2011). 'Evaluating the perceptual and pathophysiological consequences of auditory deprivation in early postnatal life: a comparison of basic and clinical studies'. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 12 (5): 535–47. doi:10.1007/s10162-011-0271-6. PMC3173557. PMID21607783.
- ^Hartley DE, Moore DR (June 2005). 'Effects of otitis media with effusion on auditory temporal resolution'. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 69 (6): 757–69. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2005.01.009. PMID15885328.
- ^Feldman, H.M.; et al. (2003). 'Parent-reported language skills in relation to otitis media during the first 3 years of life'. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 46 (2): 273–287. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2003/022). PMID14700371.
- ^Zhao F, Stephens D (1996). 'Determinants of speech-hearing disability in King-Kopetzky syndrome'. Scand Audiol. 25 (2): 91–6. doi:10.3109/01050399609047989. PMID8738633.
- ^King K, Stephens D (1992). 'Auditory and psychological factors in 'auditory disability with normal hearing''. Scand Audiol. 21 (2): 109–14. doi:10.3109/01050399209045990. PMID1641572.
- ^Jerger, James; Musiek, Frank (October 2000). 'Report of the Consensus Conference on the Diagnosis of Auditory Processing Disorders in School-Aged Children'(PDF). Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 11 (9): 467–74. PMID11057730.
- ^Working Group on Auditory Processing Disorders (2005). '(Central) Auditory Processing Disorders—The Role of the Audiologist [Position Statement]'. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. doi:10.1044/policy.PS2005-00114.
- ^Neijenhuis, Karin; de Wit, Ellen; Luinge, Margreet (13 July 2017). 'Perspectives of Dutch health professionals regarding auditory processing disorders; a focus group study'. International Journal of Audiology. 56 (12): 942–950. doi:10.1080/14992027.2017.1347290. PMID28701055.
- ^Keith, W.J., Purdy, S.C., Baily, M., Kay, Flora. Draft of New Zealand APD Guidelines on Auditory Processing Disorder (2018) Published by the Auditory Processing Disorders Reference Group, New Zealand Audiological Society.
- ^Fisher, L. I. (1976). Fisher’s Auditory Problems Checklist. Tampa, FL . The Educational Audiology Association
- ^Smoski, W.J., Brung, M.A.. and Tannahill, J.C., (1998) Children’s Auditory Performance Scale, Tampa, FL: The Educational Audiology Association
- ^Anderson, K. (1989). SIFTER: Screening instrument for targeting educational risk in children identified by hearing screening or who have known hearing loss. Tampa, FL The Educational Audiology Association
- ^O’Hara, Brian; Mealings, Kiri (31 July 2018). 'Developing the auditory processing domains questionnaire (APDQ): a differential screening tool for auditory processing disorder'. International Journal of Audiology. 57 (10): 764–775. doi:10.1080/14992027.2018.1487087. PMID30063869.
- ^Keith, R.W., Tektas, M., and Ramsay, K. (2018) Development of the University of Cincinnati Auditory Processing Inventory for Adolescents and Adults (UCAPI), American Academy of Audiology Annual Conference. April 18–21, 2018, Nashville, TN. Research Podium Presentation.
- ^Del Zoppo, C.; Sanchez, L.; Lind, C. (2015). 'A long-term follow-up of children and adolescents referred for assessment of auditory processing disorder'. International Journal of Audiology. 54 (6): 368–375. doi:10.3109/14992027.2014.972523. PMID25544358.
- ^Rintelmann, W.F. (1985). 'Monaural speech tests in the detection of central auditory disorders.'. In Marilyn L Pinheiro; Frank E Musiek (eds.). Assessment of central auditory dysfunction : foundations and clinical correlates. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. pp. 173–200. ISBN978-0-683-06887-0. OCLC11497885.
- ^Katz, Jack (1992). 'Classification of auditory processing disorders'. In Jack Katz; Nancy Austin Stecker; Donald Henderson (eds.). Central auditory processing : a transdisciplinary view. St. Louis: Mosby Year Book. pp. 81–92. ISBN978-1-55664-372-9. OCLC25877287.
- ^ ab'Central Auditory Processing: Current Status of Research and Implications for Clinical Practice. Technical Report, (1996)'. Working Group on Auditory Processing Disorders. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Archived from the original on 2007-10-04.
- ^Jerger J, Musiek F (October 2000). 'Report of the Consensus Conference on the Diagnosis of Auditory Processing Disorders in School-Aged Children'(PDF). J Am Acad Audiol. 11 (9): 467–74. PMID11057730.[permanent dead link]
- ^ abc'(Central) Auditory Processing Disorders. Technical Report, (2005)'. Working Group on Auditory Processing Disorders. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Archived from the original on 2007-06-07.
- ^Paul, Rhea (25 August 2007). 'Auditory Processing Disorder'. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 38 (1): 208–209. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0437-6. PMID17721695.
- ^ abBritish Society of Audiology (2011). 'British Society of Audiology Best Practice Guidelines'(PDF).[permanent dead link]
- ^'The British Society of Audiology and the UK APD Steering Group'.
- ^Keith, Robert W. (2000). 'SCAN-C Test for Auditory Processing Dis- orders in Children'. Revised, Psychological Corporation.
- ^Keith, Robert (2009). 'SCAN-3:A Tests for Auditory Processing Disorders in Adolescents and Adults'. Pearson US.
- ^Muluk, Nuray Bayar; Yalçınkaya, Fulya; Keith, Robert W. (February 2011). 'Random gap detection test and random gap detection test-expanded: Results in children with previous language delay in early childhood'. Auris Nasus Larynx. 38 (1): 6–13. doi:10.1016/j.anl.2010.05.007. ISSN0385-8146. PMID20599334.
- ^Keith, Robert W. (2011). 'Random gap detection test'.
- ^Musiek, Frank E.; Shinn, Jennifer B.; Jirsa, Robert; Bamiou, Doris-Eva; Baran, Jane A.; Zaida, Elena (December 2005). 'GIN (Gaps-In-Noise) Test Performance in Subjects with Confirmed Central Auditory Nervous System Involvement'. Ear and Hearing. 26 (6): 608–618. doi:10.1097/01.aud.0000188069.80699.41.
- ^Musiek, Frank (1994). 'Frequency (Pitch) and Duration Pattern Tests'. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology: 265–268.
- ^Brown, Mallory; Musiek, Frank (January 2013). 'Pathways: The Fundamentals of Masking Level Differences for Assessing Auditory Function'. The Hearing Journal. 66 (1): 16. doi:10.1097/01.HJ.0000425772.41884.1d. ISSN0745-7472.
- ^ abCacace, Anthony T.; Dennis J. McFarland (July 1995). 'Opening Pandora's Box: The Reliability of CAPD Tests'. American Journal of Audiology. 4 (2): 61–62. doi:10.1044/1059-0889.0402.61. Archived from the original on 2011-01-27. Retrieved 2010-08-31.
- ^ abCacace, Anthony T.; Dennis J. McFarland (December 2005). 'The Importance of Modality Specificity in Diagnosing Central Auditory Processing Disorder'. American Journal of Audiology. 14 (2): 112–123. doi:10.1044/1059-0889(2005/012). PMID16489868.
- ^Cacace, Anthony T.; Dennis J. McFarland (April 1998). 'Central Auditory Processing Disorder in School-Aged Children A Critical Review'. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 41 (2): 355–373. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4102.355. PMID9570588. Retrieved 2010-08-31.
- ^Dawes, P; Bishop, D.V. M. (2007). 'The SCAN-C in testing for auditory processing disorder in a sample of British children'. International Journal of Audiology. 46 (12): 780–786. doi:10.1080/14992020701545906. PMID18049967.
- ^Moore, D.R.; Ferguson, M.A.; Edmondson-Jones, A.M.; Ratib, S; Riley, A (2010). 'Nature of auditory processing disorder in children'. Pediatrics. 126 (2): e382–390. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-2826. PMID20660546.
- ^Moore, D.R.; Cowan, J.A.; Riley, A; Edmondson-Jones, A.M.; Ferguson, M.A. (2011). 'Development of auditory processing in 6-11 year old children'. Ear and Hearing. 32 (3): 269–285. doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e318201c468. PMID21233712.
- ^ abCowan J, Rosen S, Moore DR (2009). 'Putting the Auditory Processing Back into Auditory Processing Disorder in Children'. In Cacace AT, McFarland DJ (eds.). Controversies in central auditory processing disorder. San Diego, Calif.; Abingdon. pp. 187–197. ISBN978-159-756260-7.
- ^O'Connor K (December 2011). 'Auditory processing in autism spectrum disorder: A review'. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 36 (2): 836–54. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.008. PMID22155284.
- ^Jerger, James; Musick, Frank (2000). 'Report of the Consensus Conference on the Diagnosis of Auditory Processing Disorders in School-Aged Children'. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 11 (9): 467–474.
- ^Brandstaetter, Patt; Hunter, Lisa; Kalweit, Linda; Kloos, Eric; Landrud, Sherry; Larson, Nancy; Packer, Amy; Wall, Deb (2003). 'Introduction to Auditory Processing Disorders'. Minnesota Department of Education Total Special Education System.
- ^'Auditory Processing Disorder in Children [NIDCD Health Information]'. Archived from the original on 2010-12-04.
- ^DeVore, Brooke; Nagao, Kyoko; Pereira, Olivia; Nemith, Julianne; Sklar, Rachele; Deeves, Emily; Kish, Emily; Welsh, Kelsey; Morlet, Thierry (2016). 'Speech errors among children with auditory processing disorder'. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics. 29 (1): 6. doi:10.1121/2.0000440.
- ^Anderson S, Kraus N (October 2010). 'Sensory-cognitive interaction in the neural encoding of speech in noise: a review'. J Am Acad Audiol. 21 (9): 575–85. doi:10.3766/jaaa.21.9.3. PMC3075209. PMID21241645.
- ^Kamhi, A.G. (2011). 'What speech-language pathologists need to know about Auditory Processing Disorder'. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 42 (3): 265–272. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2010/10-0004). PMID20844272.
- ^Lovett, B.J. (2011). 'Auditory processing disorder: School psychologist beware?'. Psychology in the Schools. 48 (8): 855–867. doi:10.1002/pits.20595.
- ^Chermak, Gail D (2001). 'Auditory processing disorder: An overview for the clinician'. Hearing Journal. 54 (7): 10–25. doi:10.1097/01.HJ.0000294109.14504.d8.
- ^Zhao F, Stephens D (August 2000). 'Subcategories of patients with Auditory Processing Disorder'. Br J Audiol. 34 (4): 241–56. doi:10.3109/03005364000000134. PMID10997453.
- ^Subcategories of Patients with King-Kopetzky Syndrome
- ^Bellis, Teri James. 'Auditory Processing Disorders (APD) in Children'. www.asha.org. ASHA.
- ^Chermak GD, Silva ME, Nye J, Hasbrouck J, Musiek FE (May 2007). 'An update on professional education and clinical practices in central auditory processing'. J Am Acad Audiol. 18 (5): 428–52, quiz 455. doi:10.3766/jaaa.18.5.7. PMID17715652.
- ^Moore DR (2007). 'Auditory processing disorders: acquisition and treatment'. J Commun Disord. 40 (4): 295–304. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2007.03.005. PMID17467002.
- ^Moore DR, Rosenberg JF, Coleman JS (July 2005). 'Discrimination training of phonemic contrasts enhances phonological processing in mainstream school children'. Brain Lang. 94 (1): 72–85. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.11.009. PMID15896385.
- ^Russo NM, Nicol TG, Zecker SG, Hayes EA, Kraus N (January 2005). 'Auditory training improves neural timing in the human brainstem'. Behav. Brain Res. 156 (1): 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.012. PMID15474654.
- ^Alonso R, Schochat E (2009). 'The efficacy of formal auditory training in children with (central) auditory processing disorder: behavioral and electrophysiological evaluation'. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 75 (5): 726–32. doi:10.1590/S1808-86942009000500019. PMID19893943.
- ^Loo, J.H.Y.; Bamiou, D.-E.; Campbell, N.; Luxon, L.M. (2010). 'Computer-based auditory training (CBAT): benefits for children with language- and reading-related learning difficulties'. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 52 (8): 708–717. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03654.x. PMID20370814.
- ^Cameron S, Dillon H (November 2011). 'Development and Evaluation of the LiSN & Learn Auditory Training Software for Deficit-Specific Remediation of Binaural Processing Deficits in Children: Preliminary Findings'. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology. 22 (10): 678–96. doi:10.3766/jaaa.22.10.6. PMID22212767.
- ^Leite RA, Wertzner HF, Matas CG (2010). 'Long latency auditory evoked potentials in children with phonological disorder'. Pró-fono : Revista de Atualização Científica. 22 (4): 561–6. doi:10.1590/s0104-56872010000400034. PMID21271117.
- ^Mudford OC, Cullen C (2004). 'Auditory integration training: a critical review'. In Jacobson JW, Foxx RM, Mulick JA (eds.). Controversial Therapies for Developmental Disabilities. Routledge. pp. 351–62. ISBN978-0-8058-4192-3.
- ^'Is there an association between noise exposure and King Kopetzky Syndrome?'. Noise and Health. Retrieved 31 July 2010.
- ^Myklebust, H. (1954). Auditory disorders in children. New York: Grune & Stratton.
- ^Bocca E, Calearo C, Cassinari V (1954). 'A new method for testing hearing in temporal lobe tumours; preliminary report'. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 44 (3): 219–21. doi:10.3109/00016485409128700. PMID13197002.
- ^Bocca E, Calearo C, Cassinari V, Migliavacca F (1955). 'Testing 'cortical' hearing in temporal lobe tumours'. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 45 (4): 289–304. doi:10.3109/00016485509124282. PMID13275293.
- ^Kimura, Doreen (1961). 'Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli'. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 15 (3): 166–171. doi:10.1037/h0083219. ISSN0008-4255.
- ^Katz, J., & Illmer, R. (1972). Auditory perception in children with learning disabilities. In J. Katz (Ed.), Handbook of clinical audiology (pp. 540–563). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
- ^Keith, Robert W. (1977). Central auditory dysfunction: University of Cincinnati Medical Center Division of Audiology and Speech Pathology symposium. New York: Grune & Stratton. ISBN978-0-8089-1061-9. OCLC3203948.
- ^Sweetow RW, Reddell RC (1978). 'The use of masking level differences in the identification of children with perceptual problems'. J Am Audiol Soc. 4 (2): 52–6. PMID738915.
- ^Manning WH, Johnston KL, Beasley DS (February 1977). 'The performance of children with auditory perceptual disorders on a time-compressed speech discrimination measure'. J Speech Hear Disord. 42 (1): 77–84. doi:10.1044/jshd.4201.77. PMID839757.
- ^Willeford, J. A. (1977). 'Assessing central auditory behavior in children A test battery approach'. In Keith, Robert W. (ed.). Central auditory dysfunction. New York: Grune & Stratton. pp. 43–72. ISBN978-0-8089-1061-9. OCLC3203948.
- ^Jerger J, Thibodeau L, Martin J, et al. (September 2002). 'Behavioral and electrophysiologic evidence of auditory processing disorder: a twin study'. J Am Acad Audiol. 13 (8): 438–60. PMID12371661.
- ^Estes RI, Jerger J, Jacobson G (February 2002). 'Reversal of hemispheric asymmetry on auditory tasks in children who are poor listeners'. J Am Acad Audiol. 13 (2): 59–71. PMID11895008.
- ^Chermak GD, Musiek FE (2002). 'Auditory training: Principles and approaches for remediation and managing auditory processing disorders'. Seminars in Hearing. 23 (4): 287–295. doi:10.1055/s-2002-35878. ISSN0734-0451.
- ^Musiek F (June 1999). 'Habilitation and management of auditory processing disorders: overview of selected procedures'. J Am Acad Audiol. 10 (6): 329–42. PMID10385875.
- ^Task Force on Central Auditory Processing Consensus Development (1996). 'Central auditory processing: Current status of research and implications for clinical practice [Technical Report]'. American Journal of Audiology. 5: 41–54. doi:10.1044/policy.TR1996-00241. Archived from the original on 2007-10-04.
- ^Jerger J, Musiek F (October 2000). 'Report of the Consensus Conference on the Diagnosis of Auditory Processing Disorders in School-Aged Children'(PDF). J Am Acad Audiol. 11 (9): 467–74. PMID11057730. Archived from the original(PDF) on 2013-06-21. Retrieved 2012-05-24.
- ^Keith, Robert W. (1981). Central auditory and language disorders in children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press. ISBN978-0-933014-74-9. OCLC9258682.
- ^Katz, Jack; Henderson, Donald; Stecker, Nancy Austin (1992). Central auditory processing: a transdisciplinary view. St. Louis, MO: Mosby Year Book. ISBN978-1-55664-372-9. OCLC2587728.
- ^Katz, Jack; Stecker, Nancy Austin (1998). Central auditory processing disorders: mostly management. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. ISBN978-0-205-27361-4. OCLC246378171.
External links[edit]
Classification |
|
---|
Abstract
Auditory training (AT) is an important component of rehabilitation for patients with central auditory processing disorder (CAPD). The present article identifies and describes aspects of AT as they relate to applications in this population. A description of the types of auditory processes along with information on relevant AT protocols that can be used to address these specific deficits is included. Characteristics and principles of effective AT procedures also are detailed in light of research that reflects on their value. Finally, research investigating AT in populations who show CAPD or present with auditory complaints is reported. Although efficacy data in this area are still emerging, current findings support the use of AT for treatment of auditory difficulties.
Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the participant will be able to (1) describe characteristics of effective AT protocols, including aspects related to the training schedule, training difficulty, maintaining motivation, and transfer of learning and (2) describe results from current research investigating AT in patients with CAPD or who present with auditory complaints.
Central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) refers to dysfunction of the central auditory nervous system (CANS) that contributes to difficulties with perceptual processing of auditory information and that is thought to contribute to delays in skills in which successful listening serves a fundamental role. CAPD affects a variety of populations and has several suspected causes, including neuromaturational delay,1 neuroanatomical anomalies (e.g., ectopic cells), and neurologic insult of the CANS. Additionally, the term central presbycusis has recently been adopted to describe CAPD that results from changes to the CANS that occur as a result of aging. CAPD is diagnosed using a test battery comprised of behavioral, electroacoustic, and/or electrophysiologic measures that have documented sensitivity and specificity to CANS dysfunction and that assess a range of CANS processes.6 The primary complaints and symptoms of CAPD are auditory; however, due to the nonmodularity of brain organization, functional deficits can frequently manifest in related areas of attention, language, communication, and learning.7
The Listening Program For Kids
A successful treatment plan for CAPD incorporates a variety of different approaches. These approaches include environmental modifications and assistive listening devices (e.g., frequency modulation [FM] systems), development of compensatory and metacognitive strategies, delivery of necessary services for comorbid conditions, and auditory training (AT).6 The last of these approaches, AT, addresses the central auditory processing (CAP) deficit most directly by attempting to improve the function of the affected auditory process(es). A typical AT paradigm consists of challenging listening tasks that are not unlike those tests on which the patient showed difficulty during the CAPD evaluation. A patient completes these tasks several times a week while their performance on auditory processing tasks is monitored. Although additional benefits to related skills (e.g., attention) may be achieved through AT, the primary goal of enrolling a patient in AT is to minimize or eliminate the dysfunction in auditory processing.
The present article focuses on this important component of the CAPD treatment plan. We first consider auditory processing categories and general types of AT, as well as more specific AT programs. Next, we consider characteristics and parameters of AT intervention that can influence performance improvements seen over time. Finally, we review existing AT research that has been performed in populations diagnosed with CAPD or with specific auditory complaints that are not due to peripheral hearing loss.
Auditory Processes and Training
Four broad types of auditory processes are measured by the tests included in the diagnostic CAPD battery. These processes include: (1) dichotic processing, in which a different speech stimulus is simultaneously presented to each ear and the patient repeats back one or both stimuli; (2) temporal processing, which is a broad category that includes skills related to processing changes to the auditory signal over time; (3) perception of monaural low-redundancy speech, in which monaurally presented speech is degraded through filtering, the addition of noise or reverberation, and/or time compression; and (4) binaural interaction (e.g., localization, lateralization), in which complimentary inputs that differ in time, intensity, or spectral characteristics of otherwise identical stimuli are combined across the ears to support the perception of an auditory signal's spatial location. Some tests that have been used commonly to diagnose dysfunction in each of these areas include Dichotic Digits and the Competing Sentence test for dichotic processing, the Frequency Patterns test or Gaps-In-Noise for temporal processing, and the low-pass filtered speech test for perception of monaural low-redundancy speech.1013 Clinical assessment of binaural interaction is less frequently assessed by audiologists, despite the availability of the masking level difference test. More recently, spatial processing has been introduced as a fifth general category of auditory processing. This process is assessed with the listening in spatialized noise (LiSN) test that measures spatial release from masking. In its requirement that localization cues be used for the successful recognition of speech signals, this task is similar in definition, though not completely identical, to binaural interaction tasks.
Underlying each of these processes are several more fundamental abilities, including auditory discrimination and auditory identification of differences in signal frequency, intensity, and duration.6 From a psychoacoustic perspective, discrimination tasks include a same–different judgment by comparing two stimuli on the relevant acoustic dimension, and identification tasks include a judgment on which of three or more stimuli differs from the rest. Discrimination and identification are essential components of auditory processing, as they lay the foundation for more complex auditory processes that occur in the CANS.
A typical AT intervention, therefore, addresses one or more of these auditory processes and underlying skills. Both the results of the CAPD diagnostic battery, and the functional deficits presented by the patient (e.g., recognizing speech in noise) drive which areas are selected for training. The patient is administered training across clinical training sessions. During each session, performance attained during the prior session is examined. Upon attaining a particular performance criterion (typically 70 to 80% accuracy), the task is made more difficult to challenge the patient's auditory system and incrementally improve performance. At some set end point, auditory processing is reassessed clinically to determine patient progress on the training. At that point, it can be determined if additional training is needed, if the remediation plan needs to revised, or if the patient should be discharged from therapy.
Formal and Informal Auditory Training
Training typically is administered formally or informally. The distinction between these two approaches involves the level of control that is maintained over the training stimuli and the environment, and often the nature of the stimuli used. Formal training uses recorded stimuli (e.g., tones, noise, speech, digits) presented via a computer or CD player. The stimuli may be routed through an audiometer for precise control over stimulus levels, and a sound booth may be used to minimize interference from environmental sounds. Formal training also typically utilizes some mechanism for controlled adaptive difficulty. That is to say, training difficulty is modified to maintain performance at some criterion. To determine if the criterion is met, performance is scored periodically and training difficulty is modified to bring performance closer to criterion. Some types of formal training utilize a computer-based auditory training (CBAT) approach (see later).18
Conversely, informal training is typically not as concerned with stimulus control. Stimuli are presented without the use of an audiometer and may be presented face-to-face instead of using recorded stimuli. As informal training is typically done at home or in school, a sound booth is not used. Stimuli are often age-appropriate words or sentences, although nonverbal stimuli can be used in informal AT as well. Informal AT tasks typically exercises multiple auditory processes concurrently and somewhat indirectly.19
Adaptive difficulty can be achieved with informal training, though evaluation of performance relative to the criterion is typically done less frequently and is not as precise. Whenever possible, it is preferable to do formal training over informal, or to supplement formal training with informal. If it is not possible to implement formal training, then informal training may be used in isolation.
Auditory Training Software
It is becoming more commonplace for AT to be administered using software programs and, for this reason, we consider here some of the CBAT tools that clinicians and researchers are currently using to train auditory processing. Common to these programs is the presentation of training in the context of video games to keep children engaged. Some of the programs have different versions targeted toward various age ranges so that the interface for presenting these stimuli can be made age-appropriate. When CBAT programs are targeted toward adults alone, game interfaces generally are not used.
Auditory-Language Software
Earobics was one of the earliest CBAT programs. Earobics exercises underlying auditory skills in the context of auditory-language exercises (e.g., phonological awareness). Targeted auditory skills include temporal sequencing, pattern recognition, auditory closure, auditory discrimination, and auditory performance with competing signals. The program also addresses sound–symbol correspondence as it relates to reading, phonological awareness, sound blending, following oral directions, and memory and attention. Based on a review of three studies involving children diagnosed with language learning impairment or specific reading disorder, Loo and colleagues noted improved morphology, amplitudes, and latencies of speech-evoked cortical and subcortical responses in noise following Earobics training. Some positive impact was seen on phonological awareness skills; however, Earobics training had little effect on language, spelling, and reading skills of the children. Increases in the amplitude of evoked responses to speech stimuli related to brainstem and cortical substrate indicated possible improvements in electrophysiologic representation in the CANS following training with Earobics.
Fast ForWord (FFW) is a commonly used CBAT program that, similar to Earobics, targets phonological awareness and temporal processing. This program presents tasks within the context of language training; however, temporal processing underlies the theoretical foundation of FFW, which incorporates acoustic manipulations to adjust the difficulty of tasks. In their recent review of CBAT research, Loo et al reported that four FFW studies showed improvement in temporal tasks following training, with one showing no change in frequency discrimination and one showing improvement in speech-evoked cortical potentials (N1–P2). FFW demonstrated some impact on phonological awareness skills but had little effect on the language, spelling, and reading skills of children diagnosed with language learning impairment or specific reading disorder. Possible explanations for the differences across behavioral and electrophysiologic measures are discussed later.
Dichotic interaural intensity difference (DIID) training is a formal AT procedure that can be administered using customized stimuli through an audiometer or via a CBAT program.25 The procedure is intended for training dichotic processing interhemispheric transfer deficits and aims to improve performance in the weaker (dichotic) ear over time. This is accomplished by providing the weaker ear a listening advantage during dichotic training tasks by decreasing the intensity level in the stronger ear, as determined by dichotic test results (i.e., ear advantage). As the patient improves, the level in the stronger ear is increased to maintain the challenging nature of the task. In a variation of this procedure, interaural timing differences are manipulated instead of level differences to achieve the same effect.26 Two CBAT programs provide dichotic training, CAPDOTS (The Listening Academy) and the soon to be released Sound Auditory Training (SAT).27 Research with the DIID has shown that it improves dichotic listening and that gains obtained from this training are correlated with some nonauditory outcome measures, such as parent and teacher report of student listening difficulty.28
The LiSN and Learn is a CBAT program that was developed to treat spatial processing deficits identified using the LiSN test. This CBAT approach focuses on training the ability to benefit from spatially separated speech in background competition. Research on the LiSN and Learn is considered later (see “Review of Auditory Training Research Performed on Participants with Auditory Complaints”).
A new CBAT tool that is in development by the present authors is SAT. SAT is a toolkit for training a range of fundamental auditory processing skills, including auditory discrimination and identification, temporal sequencing (frequency and duration pattern recognition), gap detection, dichotic processing, binaural interaction, and auditory closure. This set of exercises allows the user to train any of the auditory processes identified by several professional consensus statements as important for listening,630 and each task is interchangeable with a range of user interfaces to promote patient interest and motivation and adaptive algorithms for training efficiency.
Characteristics of Effective Auditory Training Programs
Training Schedule
Sufficient time must be devoted to AT to induce and maintain change. Intensive therapy can require considerable time, which can be distributed in regard to the length of the training session, the number of training sessions, the time intervals between sessions, and the period of time over which training is conducted. It is common for clinicians to adopt a training schedule of three to four times a week for 20 to 30 minutes. Several studies have empirically examined training schedules. Molloy et al trained young adults on a frequency discrimination task for a total of 50 training blocks using several different training schedules: 800 trials a day for 2 days, 400 trials a day for 4 days, 200 trials a day for 8 days, and 100 trials a day for 8 days. The shortest training sessions were ∼8 minutes and the longest sessions exceeded an hour. Although all conditions yielded a similar degree of improvement following the termination of training, the shorter training sessions allowed for more latent learning, or learning that occurred between sessions. Specifically, the group that received 100 trials over 8 days improved most quickly during the early stages of training, suggesting that shorter training sessions distributed over time maximize learning efficiency. Dramatic early learning induced by AT usually is a common finding in AT research, with performance improvements generally becoming smaller over time. This finding is also supported by neurophysiological data that shows that changes emerge within 1 to 4 days of initiating AT, sometimes even preceding improvements in behavioral performance.
It should be noted that the current literature on AT does not specifically reflect on whether “booster” sessions are needed following discharge from AT to maintain auditory benefits. For instance, Anderson et al administered an 8-week program that included tasks aimed at improving: temporal order judgments of FM sweeps, discrimination of similar syllables, recognizing or matching sequences of syllables and words, implementing command sequences, and answering questions from stories. Participants showed electrophysiologic, speech-in-noise, and memory benefits; however, only the electrophysiologic benefits were maintained when assessed 6 months after training. Assessment of electrophysiologic improvements from training were maintained; however, benefits seen in speech-in-noise and memory measures were not maintained when reassessed 6 months after training. The decline in memory benefits was explained by the authors as being consistent with existing research, which shows that benefits to general cognitive abilities, such as speed of processing, tend to be maintained but benefits to more specific cognitive skills, such as working memory, do not. Furthermore, the decline in speech-in-noise benefits was interpreted by the authors as reflecting the limited short-term benefit in cognition (i.e., attention and memory skills), which over time waned. Benefits measured electrophysiologically may be more persistent because they are more purely auditory and less likely to be affected by these supramodal factors. For instance, Anderson et al note that speech-in-noise ability was assessed using the QuickSIN, and this measure may place demands on attention and memory. Because the QuickSIN benefits were not maintained, initial gains seen posttraining on this measure may have been related to short-term cognitive improvements that benefited attention and memory. Overall, these findings may suggest that booster sessions could be beneficial for maintaining AT gains.
Training Difficulty
AT tasks should be graduated in difficulty over time as a function of the patient's performance.36 Tasks should be presented systematically and employ adaptive difficulty so that the task can remain challenging and motivating, but not overwhelming. Tasks should be designed to allow patients to work at their skill threshold or edge of competency.37 The amount or degree of progression is sometimes difficult to determine. Most software programs have sufficient flexibility to adjust incremental levels; however, the question is the size step of the progression. If the step size is too large, performance will not improve, signaling that a smaller increment in difficulty level should be introduced.38 Appropriate increments in task difficulty have been shown in animal studies to be critical to improvement seen from training.
Another variable underlying AT difficulty is the targeted success-to-failure criterion ratio. Therapy programs should be designed so that the client experiences success sufficient to maintain motivation at high levels. Success rates approaching 100%, however, usually indicate that the task is too easy and that the patient's auditory system is not sufficiently challenged to elicit optimal change. On the other end of this spectrum, several studies have shown that training effects are still witnessed even when the task is impossible for the patient to complete (e.g., when the participant is asked to discriminate two identical tones). Although this would suggest that tasks cannot be made too difficult, one should be cautious of possibly demotivating the patient.
In contrast, some research with animals questions these findings. Edeline and colleagues measured behavioral and electrophysiologic changes in animals that received two types of AT for frequency discrimination, one considered to be easy and the other highly difficult (i.e., requiring discriminations beyond the capability of the animals). Interestingly, the easy AT yielded definite improvements in frequency discrimination measured behaviorally, and the difficult AT yielded essentially no improvement. Direct measurements at the auditory cortex, however, showed improved receptive field responses for both easy and difficult tasks. As noted previously in reference to the Anderson et al study, we conclude that neural timing benefits reflecting cortical auditory plasticity may be seen in the absence of certain behavioral changes, particularly when the training and/or the behavioral measures require more pervasive or more focused neural substrate that might not have been trained or reflected in the electrophysiologic measures.
Based on all findings reviewed here, we suggest that the success-to-failure ratio should be selected so that the task is challenging but not impossible to complete. To this end, a performance criterion of 70% is commonly employed,19 and task difficulty should be adaptively modified so that performance satisfies this criterion. A balanced success-to-failure criterion ratio should be targeted (sometimes referred to as the 70–30 rule) wherein the level of difficulty is adjusted to allow the patient to achieve scores of ∼70% correct and no poorer than 30%. This will help maintain motivation while providing sufficient challenge to cause change.19
Motivation and Performance Feedback
Keeping a patient motivated throughout training is an important factor in achieving successful outcomes from therapy, as it is for learning in general.43 Patients who are not motivated are not likely to be successful in an AT program. To maintain motivation, the patient must understand the rationale underlying the AT. Even children need to understand that they are enrolled in AT to improve their listening abilities, which in turn may impact their social and academic success. Teachers, parents, and clinicians should explain to children, using real-world and functional examples (e.g., ability to follow a coach's directions or understand the teacher in the noisy classroom), why they are in therapy and how it will help them.
Computer-assisted AT has grown in use due to its ability to engage participants while providing intensive training with feedback and reinforcement. Perhaps surprisingly, positive feedback can facilitate learning even when it is random in relation to a listener's responses, although excessive feedback (e.g., on 90% of trials) or no feedback at all does not contribute to learning. Thus, feedback given intermittently appears to be more effective at encouraging learning than feedback given more frequently. Motivation is related to attention. The greater the attention to a given task (such as AT), the more progress is likely to be made, and higher levels of vigilance can be maintained when the individual involved is motivated compared with unmotivated individuals. Moreover, top-down processes such as arousal and attention aid perceptual or sensory learning.
Transfer of Learning
The question as to whether AT-induced learning transfers to auditory stimuli and auditory skills not used in the training paradigm is a popular topic in AT research. As clinicians, this is a topic of significant relevance as we assume that, to some degree, administration of AT will benefit the auditory system more generally and not just for the stimuli applied. It is important for the clinician who is administering AT to consider to what degree the therapy improves outcomes that do not utilize the exact stimuli that were employed during AT (i.e., what degree of transfer of learning occurred).
Transfer of learning has been investigated within-task, that is, how well stimulus training transfers to another stimulus of a similar type for the same task, and between-task, or how well stimulus training transfers to a completely different type of auditory task that utilizes much different stimuli. Studies investigating within-task transfer effects have often examined how well training using one stimulus transfers to performance using another stimulus that has a slightly different acoustic characteristic from the first. Training participants on a duration discrimination task has been shown to generalize to improvements for stimuli with durations not used during the training. Similarly, training on a frequency discrimination task also tends to generalize to frequencies not used during the training, at least after several administrations of the untrained frequencies.
Research examining between-task transfer effects have frequently addressed to what degree training on basic auditory processing tasks transfers to more complex skills. For instance, Kujala et al examined whether a combined nonspeech temporal processing and auditory discrimination AT would generalize to skills like reading. They noted that the trained group showed a significant increase in the number of words and the reading rate. Moore et al trained participants on a phoneme discrimination task and examined to what degree this training generalized to receptive language skills. They noted that, as a group, phonological awareness and word discrimination scores did improve following training.
There certainly are situations, however, in which transfer of learning does not appear to occur. Millward and colleagues trained participants on either a frequency discrimination task in quiet or in the presence of modulated noise, or on words in modulated noise. All trained groups, and even a control group that was untrained, showed some improvements on a words-in-noise probe; however, frequency discrimination improvements were seen only in subjects who were included in one of the frequency discrimination training groups. The authors concluded that, in general, if the training stimulus shares some dimension with the outcome measure, then training benefits are more likely to be seen. This conclusion was supported by further research showing that learning does not always generalize across stimuli or tasks.
Transfer of learning, when it occurs, almost certainly makes use of nonauditory specific skills (i.e., supramodal skills) or what others have called procedural or conceptual learning. Delhommeau et al discussed transfer of learning in the context of meta-learning, or learning about the basic nature of the AT task. Learning in this way would be expected to recruit cognitive skills that contribute to within-task transfer of learning. Moore et al also emphasized the benefits that AT provides to attention and how these improvements in attention could facilitate transfer of learning to tasks and skills not applied during the training paradigm. Evidence from this viewpoint is provided by research that has shown that training on a visual task (which does not recruit the auditory system at all) can lead to improvements in auditory discrimination.
Process-Specific Training
A related concept is what has been called “process-specific training.” It has been recommended that AT be process-specific when administered to patients with CAPD. That is, the auditory process(es) shown to be deficient should be targeted in AT.6 To perform process-specific training, a full CAPD diagnostic evaluation must be completed prior to undertaking AT with older children (i.e., age 7 to 8 years and older) and adults. The diagnostic test battery should provide information about the patient's particular auditory strengths and weaknesses. As many clinics that perform CAP evaluations today do not test a full range of central auditory capabilities (e.g., dichotic processing, temporal processing, perception of monaural low redundancy speech, and binaural interaction), an incomplete profile of the patient's auditory strengths and weaknesses is often generated, making it difficult to decide precisely what skills to train. Research is not available regarding the considerations that actually guide clinicians' specific decisions when creating a CAPD test battery, although it is frequently recommended that sensitivity and specificity data from CAPD tests and test batteries be prioritized in this context.
Review of Auditory Training Research Performed on Participants with Auditory Complaints
Although there is a fairly large body of research investigating AT (or auditory-language training) in children with speech-language and/or learning disabilities, very few studies have examined the effectiveness of AT in children diagnosed with CAPD or some form of nonperipheral auditory deficit. Studies that look at training-based remediation in individuals with auditory complaints are important for a variety of reasons. Foremost among these is that such studies speak specifically to whether these paradigms benefit the very individuals toward which these therapies are targeted. To this end, the present section provides a more detailed discussion of those research studies that have examined AT in individuals with primarily auditory complaints.
Each of the studies below is considered from the perspective of the level of evidence that applies. Using terminology reported in Appendix A of the American Academy of Audiology (AAA) CAPD practice guidelines, level 1 includes the most rigorous studies (e.g., double-blind, randomized clinical trials, etc.), level 2 includes quasi-experimental research (e.g., nonrandomized, retrospective designs with control groups, etc.), level 3 includes observational studies with controls (e.g., case studies, cohort studies, etc.), level 4 includes descriptive studies (i.e., observational without controls), and level 5 reflects expert clinical opinion, consensus, or standards for practice.6 Each of the studies discussed below are listed in Table 1 along with the respective level of evidence. In some cases, the classification into level of evidence is imperfect given that the research did not exactly fit the definition for any level. In these cases, the level that best described the research was selected.
Table 1
Study | Level of Evidence* |
---|---|
Sharma et al60 | 1 |
Krishnamurti et al61 | 4 |
McArthur et al59 | 4 |
Moncrieff and Wertz68 | 4 |
Musiek et al26 | 4 |
Weihing et al70 | 3 |
Cameron and Dillon29 | 4 |
Musiek and Schochat25 | 4 |
Putter-Katz et al74 | 2 |
Alonso and Schochat75 | 4 |
Schochat et al76 | 2 |
Musiek and Baran77 | 4 |
Musiek et al78 | 4 |
Musiek et al79 | 4 |
Note: Studies are listed in the order that they are cited in the text.
Temporal Processing Training and Auditory Discrimination Training
Temporal processing represents a broad category of auditory processes, including skills such as temporal sequencing, temporal integration, temporal resolution, and others. Additional skills that are fundamental to temporal processing are frequency, intensity, and duration discrimination. AT paradigms that target temporal processing are generally varied in their focus and scope given the range of temporal processes that can be addressed. Some of these paradigms are considered next.
Sharma et al recruited 55 children (7 to 13 years, mean = 9.7 years) who were diagnosed with CAPD according to the AAA guidelines. Subjects all had normal peripheral hearing sensitivity and were randomly assigned to one of the following conditions: auditory discrimination training (AT), auditory discrimination training and FM system, language therapy, language therapy and FM system, or a nonintervention control group. Training entailed a total of 12 hours of intervention that included both home-based and clinic-based tasks. The 5-week training consisted of 1-hour clinical sessions weekly and 15 minutes of exercises at home for 5 days a week. The exercises performed at home consisted of some of the AT games included in Earobics, in particular the tasks that focused on phonological training. For the language therapy group, home exercises consisted of the following: reading aloud while emphasizing correct stress and intonation, and focusing on appreciating differences in meaning that are conveyed from the use of different stress and timing patterns. The findings indicated that all interventions yielded some degree of improvement. Both the auditory discrimination training and the language therapy groups showed posttraining improvements in temporal processing. These groups showed additional benefits in language and reading outcomes. Although these results might suggest that either AT or language therapy can yield the same results, it is generally expected that training that specifically focuses on an affected auditory process(es) will most likely produce greatest benefits, as described earlier. Because the AT paradigm did not necessarily address processes shown to be affected by the CAPD test battery, lesser gains might have shown by these participants than if a process-specific AT approach had been adopted. It should be noted that a control group that did not receive any interventions did not show significant improvement on measures over time.
Krishnamurti et al examined the effectiveness of FFW in two cases diagnosed with CAPD. As noted earlier, FFW is a computer-based AT paradigm that targets temporal processing and auditory discrimination skills in a speech-language context. The two pediatric cases (7 to 8 years old) presented normal peripheral hearing sensitivity and showed difficulties on the researchers' central auditory test battery. The battery consisted of one highly sensitive and specific nonverbal measure of CAPD (i.e., Frequency Patterns), as well as several auditory language-based measures that more generally involve central auditory aspects as well (i.e., SCAN-C, Phonemic Synthesis,63 and TAPS-R64). The children also were administered measures to look at an electrophysiologic measure of auditory processing, as well as language or more general supramodal skills. These measures included the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI),65 Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF),66 and the speech auditory brainstem response (seABR). The children participated in this AT paradigm over an 8- to 12-week period, 5 days a week, for 50-minute sessions.
Results showed that the first participant improved on the FFW tasks, which was expected given the intensity of the training. This participant also demonstrated gains on the SCAN, Frequency Patterns, Phonemic Synthesis, and the CELF. Benefits seemed to generalize beyond the central auditory and language measures, as indicated by improvements in cognition as measured by the TONI. Improvements also were observed electrophysiologically on the seABR, which was seen as an increase in amplitude of the V-A response (i.e., peak-to-peak amplitude from wave V to the negativity that follows wave V). The second participant showed similar improvements following training on the auditory processing measures and the seABR (i.e., shorter latencies); however, no notable improvements were seen on the TONI or the CELF. These findings should be interpreted with caution as they reflect the results of only two subjects.
McArthur and coworkers recruited 28 children (6 to 15 years old) who presented with a specific reading disability or a specific language impairment and who also demonstrated below-normal performance on temporal processing measures. Although a CAPD battery was not administered, the reduced performance on tests of temporal processing indicated this sample experienced difficulties on auditory processing tasks. Children were trained on one of the following auditory discrimination or temporal processing tasks: frequency discrimination, vowel discrimination, consonant–vowel discrimination, or backward masking. Training, which was designed to be adaptive in difficulty, was administered for 30 minutes a day, 4 days a week, for 6 weeks. Psychoacoustic tasks that were similar to the AT exercises were administered before the AT paradigm was started and then a second time following the last training session. Approximately 90% of the participants demonstrated performance on these measures that was within normal limits following the last training session. The improvements shown did not appear to be explained by a more global change in how participants approached the tasks. This was evidenced by the finding that participants did not show improvements for two nonauditory tasks: visual discrimination and sustained attention tasks. Additionally, subjects in a control condition who were not exposed to the training also demonstrated some degree of improvement on outcome measures when administered at retest; however, the magnitudes of these improvements were smaller than that seen from the trained group. Of note was that the control participants generally demonstrated better temporal processing skills than the trained group when measured at the pretraining session and, therefore, did not have as much potential for improvement as the experimental group.
Dichotic Auditory Training
Several studies have examined the effectiveness of dichotic training in participants with dichotic processing issues as confirmed using CAPD tests. Moncrieff and Wertz administered dichotic therapy (“Auditory Rehabilitation for Interaural Asymmetry”) to children with dichotic deficits in two studies. The dichotic issues exhibited by the participants were either a unilateral left ear weakness or a bilateral weakness. In both studies, dichotic stimuli were presented through two speakers in the sound field, and difficulty was adjusted by increasing the intensity of the acoustic signal coming from the right speaker over time. The initial interaural intensity difference was 30 dB; as patients gradually demonstrated an improvement in performance, this interaural intensity difference was decreased across sessions by 1 to 5 dB to maintain performance between 70 and 100%. Eight children ranging in age between 7 and 13 years participated in the first study reported by Moncrieff and Wertz. The children trained for 30 minutes per session, three sessions a week, for 4 weeks. This study represented a phase I and phase II clinical trials in which a treatment is investigated in a smaller and then larger group of participants, respectively, to examine the treatment's ability to yield improvements on outcomes. Control groups were not included as part of the phase I and phase II clinical trials.
Findings indicated that left ear performance for dichotic digits improved significantly following training, with the average observed improvement of ∼15%. In their second study, Moncrieff and Wertz recruited a larger sample of children (6.5 to 11 years, n = 13). In addition to a larger sample, some participants in study 2 trained over a longer duration (ranging from 12 to 24 sessions), and they were also given additional nondichotic listening comprehension outcome measures to assess the generalization of benefits. In this second study, significant left and right ear improvements posttraining were seen for both dichotic digits and competing words measures, with an approximate 20% improvement in performance for the left ear. Although improvements in the right ear were much smaller in magnitude, pretraining performance for the right ear generally was much higher than the left ear score, which limited the degree of improvement that could obtained. This observation relates to a common trend seen in AT studies where individuals with poorer baseline performance generally obtain greater gains from AT. A significant correlation was obtained between improvements in left ear scores on both dichotic measures and the listening comprehension measures (i.e., Brigance Comprehensive Inventory of Basic Skills Revised).
Musiek et al reported AT data in which a DIID-like therapy was administered to 14 children with dichotic issues for 10 weeks.26 Although the paradigm typically attempts to improve weaker ear performance by manipulating interaural level differences, this study utilized interaural timing differences instead. The authors referred to this variant of the DIID paradigm as the DIID II. By having the stimulus in the weaker ear arrive slightly later in time than the stimulus in the better ear, the weaker ear obtains a dichotic processing advantage (i.e., the lag effect). As the patient improves over time, the lag between the ears is gradually decreased to make the task more challenging. In this particular study, no control group was recruited.
The authors reported a significant improvement in pre- versus posttraining dichotic listening scores that was ∼30% in magnitude. Interestingly, improvements in dichotic processing also appeared to be related to a reduction of everyday symptoms, as determined by parent or teacher report. Questionnaire respondents were asked about the patient's improvements in their ability to follow directions, communication ability, academic performance, attention, and ability to hear in noise. Scores ranged from 0 (no improvement) to 5 (100% improvement). Examined collectively, the trained group demonstrated average or greater than average improvement.
Weihing et al administered the DIID to four older adults, two who demonstrated dichotic deficits and two who showed normal dichotic processing.70 The DIID was performed over ∼20 sessions using a range of stimuli and instructions. Following the termination of training, adults with dichotic deficits showed improvement and, in some cases, normal dichotic processing. Furthermore, the subjects with dichotic deficits showed improved performance on the QuickSIN posttraining, and those without deficits did not improve on this measure as much. These initial results are encouraging, suggesting the utility of the DIID to patients with central presbycusis. The findings must be tempered, however, by the small sample size and the fact that the older adults without dichotic deficits were younger by ∼8 years and showed better peripheral hearing than the older adults with dichotic deficits.
Spatial Processing
As mentioned above, the LiSN and Learn was developed to treat spatial processing disorder. To evaluate the utility of this approach, Cameron and Dillon enrolled nine children (6 to 11 years) to participate in the LiSN and Learn program, 15 to 20 minutes a day, five times a week, for ∼3 months. Results revealed that participants were better able to make use of spatial separation cues following training and that these improvements persisted 3 months posttraining. Significant benefits were also noted on some measures of attention and memory, as well as on report measures of hearing handicap. Although LiSN and Learn appears to provide benefits to children with this type of processing issue when they have normal peripheral hearing, the training does not appear to be as effective in cases of sensorineural hearing loss.
Battery Approach to Training
Several studies have investigated whether a battery of treatment approaches for individuals diagnosed with CAPD or auditory complaints provides benefits to this clinical group. The training battery approach includes multiple types of AT exercises, spanning a range of auditory processes, and may also make use of top-down instruction and compensatory modifications. Although the training battery approach described below does not always target processes that were shown to be deficient, this approach has some ecological validity as individuals with CAPD typically present with issues related to more than one auditory process or ability. Moreover, difficulties with multiple auditory processes are frequently thought to underlie listening deficits (e.g., listening difficulty in noise might be due to binaural separation issues, spatial processing issues, temporal processing issues, etc.). Thus, a training battery approach may be beneficial when the affected auditory process cannot be easily identified or isolated. This might occur, for instance, if electrophysiologic measures were used to diagnosis an auditory processing deficit.
Musiek and Schochat reported a case study in which a 15-year-old with CAPD received benefits from an AT battery.25 Diagnosis of CAPD was made using four tests: Dichotic Digits, Compressed Speech, Frequency Patterns, and Duration Patterns. Prior to training, the child showed difficulties on all of the CAPD measures with the exception of the Duration Patterns. The clinical AT protocol followed a schedule of 1-hour sessions, three times a week, for 6 weeks. Additional exercises also were provided informally so that participants could engage in therapy at home. These informal exercises were completed for 15 to 30 minutes, two to three times a week. Formal training tasks provided in the clinic focused on auditory discrimination (i.e., intensity discrimination, frequency discrimination, CV discrimination), dichotic processing (i.e., DIID training), and monaural low-redundancy (i.e., speech-in-noise training). Difficulty level of these tasks was adaptive to maintain performance at 70% correct. Informal training tasks performed at home included such tasks as reading aloud with good intonation and rhythm (which is a top-down approach that targets discrimination and temporal processing) and identification of target lyrics in songs.
When the training paradigm was completed, the participant had improved or attained normal CAP, particularly for the compressed speech task and dichotic processing measures. The participant's parent completed a hearing questionnaire that targeted the degree of improvement received from the training, rated from 0 (no improvement) to 5 (no longer has a problem in this area). Target behaviors included following directions, communication ability, academic ability, attention span, ability to recognize speech recognition in noise, and alertness. The parent indicated that the participant showed considerable (score of 3) to marked improvement (score of 4) in these areas. The range of areas in which improvements were noted following training may speak to the broad scope of the skills targeted in this AT paradigm and/or to the primacy of CAPD in contributing to the patient's symptoms.
Putter-Katz and colleagues used dichotic listening and speech-in-noise tasks in an AT paradigm and coupled this approach with the fitting of an FM system, in addition to top-down interventions (e.g., modification of learning strategies, cognitive and metacognitive approaches, and classroom and home modifications). Participants included 20 children, ranging in age from 7 to 14 years (mean = 9 years), and 10 control subjects who ranged in age from 6 to 11 years (mean = 8 years). The control subjects received neither training nor intervention. Both groups were diagnosed with CAPD based on the following criteria: performance below 1 standard deviation in at least one ear on one or more of the central auditory tests used in the study. These central auditory tests included dichotic listening (binaural separation), monaural low-redundancy, temporal processing (i.e., gap detection ability), and binaural interaction. All participants presented with listening difficulties, as noted by caregivers, parents, and teachers. Examination of the types of deficits seen in this group revealed that 11 children were diagnosed with monaural low-redundancy deficits only (i.e., “noise group”), and 9 children were diagnosed with deficits on tests that assess monaural low-redundancy speech perception and dichotic processing skills (i.e., “noise + dichotic group”). A training paradigm was administered for 45 minutes, once a week, for 4 months. Outcome measures included the central auditory tests administered prior to training. Following 4 months of training, central auditory test scores increased for participants in both the “noise” and “noise + dichotic” groups. The control group did not show similar improvements.
Alonso and Schochat recruited 29 children with CAPD who ranged in age from 8 to 16 years to determine the utility of AT. The diagnostic CAPD battery included two measures of monaural low-redundancy and two measures of dichotic processing. The CAPD diagnosis was made based on the participant failing at least two CAPD tests in the battery. These tests and the auditory P300 served as outcome measures for the study. The training schedule consisted of eight, 50-minute sessions, once per week for 8 weeks. The training paradigm was similar to that employed by Musiek and Schochat (described above).25 Task difficulty was modified each week to maintain performance at ∼70%. At the end of the training, significant improvements were observed on most of the outcome measures, including P300 latency and all CAPD tests. Nearly 73% of the participants presented normal auditory processing ability following training.
Schochat et al recruited 30 children with CAPD and 22 without CAPD for an AT study. Participants ranged in age from 8 to 14 years and were diagnosed with CAPD using the criteria set forth by American Speech-Language-Hearing Association and AAA.630 None of the participants was diagnosed with peripheral hearing loss. The children diagnosed with CAPD were enrolled in training for 8 weeks, with 50-minute sessions, once weekly. Training tasks were comprised of a combination of frequency discrimination, intensity discrimination, duration discrimination, gap detection, DIID, localization, and speech perception training. Informal exercises consisted of word recognition tasks that were given to subjects to be completed at home for 15 minutes daily. Posttraining outcome measures consisted of speech recognition in quiet and in noise, verbal and nonverbal dichotic listening, and the auditory middle latency response. Participants exhibited significant improvement on all central auditory tests following training. Additionally, the children who received training also showed an increase in the amplitude of the middle latency response when it was measured over the left hemisphere. This electrophysiologic enhancement was not seen in the control group.
Several case studies with patients with neurologic symptoms also have examined the effectiveness of AT using a battery of auditory exercises and interventions. Musiek and Baran reported on a young adult patient who experienced a hemorrhage in the pons that was a consequence of an arteriovenous malformation. The patient was evaluated audiologically at 3 months following this event, at which point she had noted significant hearing difficulties, especially when listening in background noise. An audiogram performed at this visit showed relatively normal hearing in the left ear and a sensorineural loss that was severe in the high frequencies in the right ear. Two monaural low-redundancy tests also were administered. Performance on these tests was 0% in the right ear and ranged between 50 and 75% in the left ear. The patient was enrolled in an informal rehabilitation protocol, which included the following recommendations: wear an earplug in the right ear to prevent distortion, use an assistive listening device, and participate in AT. The patient was enrolled in AT for ∼15 to 20 minutes daily for 6 months. AT consisted of auditory discrimination of numbers, consonants, vowels, words, and sentences. These tasks were administered over the telephone (to simulate a monaural low-redundancy, filtered speech context) with a friend reading the stimuli to the patient. During each AT session, training began with the better ear and then proceeded to the poorer right ear. Her performance on CAPD tests was reassessed twice during the course of training. Subjectively, the patient noted significant improvements in listening, although listening in noisy situations was still challenging. Her right ear performance on one of the monaural low-redundancy tests demonstrated improvement, from 0 to ∼50%. Interestingly, the high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss also showed improvement, progressing from a severe loss to mild to moderate loss. It should be noted though that this improvement was likely seen because a portion of her hearing loss was related to central changes that may have been coincident with the hemorrhage in her pons. In general, it would not be expected that AT would improve peripheral hearing loss.
In another case, Musiek and coworkers reported on a 21-year-old patient with a subarachnoid bleed that involved the inferior colliculi bilaterally. Initially, the patient showed a complete central hearing loss and was unresponsive to sounds. Physiologic measures showed normal otoacoustic emissions and auditory brainstem response through wave III. Over the course of 12 weeks, improvements were seen in his hearing sensitivity, and by 10 months his hearing sensitivity was equivalent to a moderately severe hearing loss. The patient continued to experience difficulties hearing in noise even though his hearing sensitivity had improved. Intensity discrimination was measured at 5 and 11 weeks postinsult. At both sessions, the difference limen to intensity was elevated, though it was slightly improved at 11 weeks for the right ear and for binaural administration. Electrophysiologic measures, including the middle latency response and event-related potentials, were also abnormal, further confirming involvement of the central auditory system. An auditory treatment plan was put into place 1 month after the patient left the hospital. The AT paradigm was administered formally, once a week for approximately an hour, for 14 weeks. Training was adaptive to maintain difficulty at a moderate level. Additionally, informal exercises were given for home use and these included auditory directives, discrimination tasks, and music listening. Initially, the formal training consisted of the following: having the patient answer questions about himself, administration of discrimination tasks, and speech reading. When the treatment paradigm was ∼50% complete, the therapy focus changed to identification and discrimination of voices and environmental sounds, speech recognition in noise, identification of nonword speech sounds, and reading aloud. The patient made clear progress in auditory discrimination ability throughout these latter stages of therapy. Toward the end of treatment, an emphasis was placed on significantly increasing the difficulty of therapy because the patient had, to this point, shown large gains in performance. Discrimination tasks included sounds that were more similar, and more complex sentence-level material was introduced. An assistive listening device also was dispensed around this time. It should be noted that Musiek et al acknowledged it was difficult to separate out benefits obtained from AT from those obtained from spontaneous recovery.
Musiek and colleagues also reported an AT case in which hearing difficulties were noted following a mild head trauma. The 41-year-old patient reported difficulty attending for longer periods of time, especially when auditory information was presented. She also observed difficulty understanding multistep directions and felt she had more difficulty hearing from the left ear than the right ear, despite showing normal and symmetrical hearing sensitivity bilaterally. A CAPD evaluation that included Dichotic Digits, Competing Sentences, Frequency Patterns, Duration Patterns, and Compressed Speech revealed that the patient's performance was consistent with a diagnosis of CAPD. She was enrolled in an AT program that included training in advocating for clear speech, reading aloud, DIID training, auditory memory enhancement training, auditory speech discrimination training, temporal sequence training, and instruction on metacognitive strategies. Posttraining, the patient showed significant gains in CAP: performance on the dichotic digits and right ear scores on the compressed speech and competing sentences were now within normal limits. Significant improvements also were seen in the left ear on compressed speech and competing sentences, though performance only bordered normal limits.
Conclusions
The present article has described AT approaches for the treatment of CAPD. Training focused on one or more auditory processes, including temporal processes, dichotic processing, perception of monaurally presented low-redundancy signals, and binaural interaction, including the related spatial processing. Several AT (and auditory-language) exercises are available for administration via the computer (i.e., CBAT). CBAT has many advantages over face-to-face training, including controlled presentation of stimuli, precise implementation of adaptive difficulty, intensiveness (i.e., number of trials per session), and engaging and reinforcing interfaces. Characteristics of successful AT include application of appropriate training schedules, use of adaptive difficulty, and presentation of reinforcement to maintain motivation. Transfer of learning is known to occur, though the magnitude of the transfer effect is larger for tasks that are more similar to the trained task. Although published research generally shows a positive impact of AT in this population, future CAPD training research should strive to include control groups (or control as well as target dependent variables in single-subject research) wherever possible and focus on the degree to which benefits generalize to listening skills not trained during the AT paradigm. The skills assessed should be related to the presenting symptoms of patients with CAPD, such as difficulty hearing in noise, and might be measured using behavioral and electrophysiologic measures, well as validated, informant (e.g., parent) report measures. Additionally, future research should be designed to be more consistent with phase III clinical trials in which control groups are generally recruited. Finally, it is often recommended that AT take a process-specific approach, in which the processes shown to be affected by CAPD are targeted by the training paradigm. At this time, the scientific literature does not establish that this approach yields larger gains than a more general training protocol (e.g., AT training battery), which is not necessarily process specific.